Biologists increasingly use marketing strategies to promote their results, obtain funding, and influence decisionmakers, but this development can compromise public perceptions of their objectivity and prevent the effective communication of reliable scientific knowledge. Responsible and effective communication is particularly important in this era of biotechnological innovation and global environmental change, when knowledge is often uncertain and rapidly evolving and can have huge consequences for society. Although scientists and science journalists carefully evaluate scientific results, they more freely choose rhetorical elements and, in particular, their metaphors, despite the value-laden judgments that often accompany these choices. We therefore argue that metaphors should be carefully chosen and evaluated alongside empirical evidence, because they shape data interpretation and how science influences society. Here, we propose guidelines for the responsible use of metaphors in science writing and communication.

Scientists are increasingly advised to use marketing strategies and strong metaphors or dominant news frames to get their message across (Bubela et al. 2009, Larson 2011, Kuchner 2012, Nelder 2013), because there is growing competition for funding, public attention, and the ability to influence decisionmaking (e.g., Fischer et al. 2012). We think that this development creates problems to the extent that it invites scientists to undermine their objectivity by making assertions that are not based entirely on data. Instead, when scientists make public statements that build on their legitimacy as neutral and knowledgeable experts, their appropriate role is to act as “honest brokers of policy alternatives” (Pielke 2007). They can contribute to controversial debates and express their personal opinion, but, to the extent that this is possible, they should explain the degree of subjectivity in their opinion and provide alternative ways to interpret the evidence.

Decisionmaking in our time depends on reliable scientific evidence, so neglecting or misinterpreting such evidence may impair decisionmaking. However, knowledge is often uncertain or incomplete. In these instances, incipient knowledge can be used to substantiate multiple conflicting views on an issue, thereby contributing to policy impasses (Sarewitz 2004, Kahan et al. 2012). As biologists, we are increasingly confronted with such situations, especially in an era of biotechnological innovation and global environmental change in which much knowledge that once seemed solid is fundamentally changing. This puts a great burden of responsibility on science communication, so media coverage of scientific findings is rightly expected to follow strict guidelines of good practice and ethical behavior. Trustworthy science journalists focus on peer-reviewed literature, carefully fact check articles, and report not only primary research results but also critical assessments by peers in the research field. Scientists are equally concerned that the interpretations and limitations of their science are presented in a correct and evidence-based way.

In this article, however, we highlight an aspect of responsible reporting of scientific evidence that has received less attention. It appears that, although scientists and science journalists carefully evaluate scientific results and ways to interpret them, they more freely choose metaphors to communicate to their audience (see Nerlich et al. 2009). In this manner, they may communicate interpretations and weightings of scientific results that are not supported by data. Although there is no such thing as value-free communication (Weber and Word 2001), we argue that scientists and journalists should reflect carefully on their communication choices to minimize confusion and contested interpretations of uncertain knowledge.

The performative quality of language at the research frontier

We focus on metaphors here because they are linguistic choices that have been shown to shape the thinking of scientists and, therefore, the direction of research fields (Keller 2002, Brown 2003, Larson 2011). They are, in this sense, performative—meaning that they lead to actual outcomes in the world. Accordingly, a narrow choice of metaphorical tools can restrict the diversity of research approaches and creative thinking, especially at emerging research frontiers (Larson 2009). This is of particular concern when research outputs can have huge consequences for society.

For instance, scientific and popular articles about research in synthetic biology often build on metaphors taken from the worlds of machines, computers, or consumerism (Cserer and Seiringer 2009, Keller 2009, Hellsten and Nerlich 2011). The titles of recent articles in New Scientist, for example, proclaim “Evolution machine: Genetic engineering on fast forward,” “Genetic code 2.0: Life gets a new operating system,” “Synthetic biology's Ikea,” and “Synthetic ‘upgrade’ for fruit fly's DNA.” These titles often encapsulate the core metaphor of an article, but such metaphors are usually prevalent throughout a text. Elsewhere, for example, individuals affiliated with syntheticbiology.org, which was initiated by students, faculty, and staff from Massachusetts Institute of Technology and Harvard, seek to “specify and populate a set of standard parts that have well-defined performance characteristics and can be used (and re-used) to build biological systems,” “reverse engineer and re-design pre-existing biological parts and devices in order to expand the set of functions that we can access and program” (http://syntheticbiology.org/FAQ.html), and “construct… a genetic code with an enlarged alphabet of base pairs” (http://syntheticbiology.org/Who_we_are.html). The use, here, of mechanistic language implies an ease of manipulation and a certainty of understanding that misrepresents the knowledge of these biological systems (Boudry and Pigliucci 2013), as well as what synthetic biologists actually do in their research and how they think about the associated risks (Cserer and Seiringer 2009, Marris C and Rose 2012). Furthermore, the implied ability to control biological systems prioritizes an instrumental way of valuing life, in contrast to an intrinsic or even sacred one (Deplazes-Zemp 2012). Given the holistic and dynamic nature of the genome and the cell (Boudry and Pigliucci 2013), we recommend following ecology and evolutionary biology and, instead, using metaphors such as those of tangled genome (compare tangled bank in Darwin 1869) or tinkering instead of design (Jacob 1977), because they more fully acknowledge organismal complexity (Proctor and Larson 2005). Using such alternative metaphors could change the way research is done in synthetic biology and how it is taken up by industry and the general public.

There are many other prominent metaphors in biology that have gained disproportionate importance and that shape both research and social outcomes (see Larson 2011 for a review). The metaphor of ecosystem services, for example, currently dominates ecological research on human–
environment relationships, thereby reinforcing a perspective from economic production that emphasizes the direct benefits that ecosystems provide to humans while neglecting alternative conceptions that are focused on stewardship and moral duty, long-term sustainability, and the cultural values of ecosystems (Raymond et al. 2013). Similar examples can be given for other biological research fields, such as health (e.g., Sontag 1989) or neuroscience (e.g., Slaney and Maraun 2005). The problem here is not so much that a metaphor is wrong but that it is misleading: It encourages the interpretation of a partial view as the whole truth or the attribution of too much importance to the view provided by one metaphor as opposed to the different insights provided by a plurality of them.

Such a tunneling of our intellectual engagement with an emerging topic is problematic for a number of reasons. It weakens creativity and threatens our ability to foresee unknown problems and risks (“unknown unknowns”), and it also excludes members of society who do not share the implicit values of the dominant narrative, which can lead to public opposition and controversy (Marris C and Rose 2012). Excluding people in this way undermines responsible scientific conduct, which requires scientists to ensure that their research contributes to democratic deliberations that serve the common good (Kitcher 2004).

Inclusive and effective communication with the public

Effective communication with the public is a rapidly growing concern for scientific fields ranging from biotechnology to ecology (Beardsley 2006, Bubela et al. 2009, Groffman et al. 2010, Nerlich et al. 2010), and metaphors are often used to help readers connect to scientific results. Although we do not question the usefulness of metaphors in science communication, we turn to the example of invasive species to highlight how value-laden messaging can also hinder effective communication and action.

Metaphors of war are often used when reporting about nonnative invasive species (Larson 2005, 2011), as well as in other environmental sciences (Hamblin 2013). Popular and scientific articles abound that include terms such as enemy, eradication, fight, and war or that liken biological invasions to bioterrorism. However, there is a growing consensus in the literature about invasive species that such language hinders effective engagement with diverse stakeholders and nature conservation practitioners (Young and Larson 2011), especially in situations in which the scientific information is ambiguous, uncertain, or incomplete (Larson et al. 2013). Indeed, researchers in the field think about the problem in diverse ways, which leads to differing assessments of the negative effects of nonnative species and the urgency of the problem (Humair et al. 2014). In particular, the positive effects of nonnative species are increasingly acknowledged (e.g., Schlaepfer et al. 2011), so popular scientific articles portray them as beneficial and encourage readers to “embrace invasives” (Vince 2011) or “welcome weeds… [that] could save the Earth” (Hamilton 2011). Trapped within an either–or duality, these authors overstate this alternative view so that it becomes as misleading as the view that all nonnative species are bad. The extreme of battling invasives may encourage managers to take rigorous action under all circumstances without considering alternatives and balancing implementation costs, opportunity costs, and benefits (Larson 2005), just as the other extreme of embracing invasives may encourage them to ignore the problem rather than to seek solutions. Using such hyperbole to report on an issue that is actually quite nuanced can lead to confusion and may hinder the responsible and effective use of scientific evidence in decisionmaking and the media. In contrast, neutral and balanced language will help ensure that policymakers and scientific advisers are not in the difficult situation of needing to overcome stereotypes when seeking solutions to the problem that adequately address its sociopolitical and biological complexities.

The metaphors in scientific texts should be used to help readers understand scientific findings, not to convince them without explaining the reasons. The International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) has, for instance, recently listed the giant salvinia (Salvinia molesta) as one of the world's 100 worst invasive species and reported this in Nature in a letter entitled “Alien species: Monster fern makes IUCN invader list” (Courchamp 2013). We consider this choice of words to be undesirable, because it merely expresses a value judgment of the authors (i.e., that the species is like a monster because it is bad) rather than illustrating the science. The metaphor devalues this plant species in its entirety (like a monster that is always bad) rather than specifying which aspects of its behavior are problematic. In short, the monster metaphor implies an absolute value judgment, which may undermine the public's perception of science as a critical and balanced form of inquiry and, ultimately, their trust in it. Given their societal role, scientists should make value statements such as this one in a more explicit way—for example, by calling the fern a problem rather than a monster. Or they might use a metaphor to explain the underlying science rather than to express an opinion—for example, by calling this aquatic fern that rapidly proliferates in water polluted by nutrients a gourmand fern.

Strong messaging can undermine the credibility of ­science—in particular, when contradictory facts become available and, therefore, scientific uncertainties become visible to a broader public. For instance, ecologists adopted apocalyptic warnings in the Waldsterben (dying forest) debate in the 1970s and 1980s in Europe, and this choice still undermines their expert status several decades later (Horeis 2009). It might seem that engagement with uncertainties will reduce the capacity for science to contribute to decisionmaking, and there is undoubtedly political demand for clear and fixed answers. In the long run, however, ­scientists and science journalists need to reiterate that science is not so much a collection of established facts as a process of inquiry; therefore, especially in the case of contested and complex societal issues, responsible science communication entails stating the facts in neutral language and acknowledging areas of uncertainty (Pielke 2007). Conversely, a major concern for scientists is that journalists tend to portray a scientifically undisputed case—such as the anthropogenic causes of climate change—as an unresolved issue by giving unjustified space to extremists’ views (e.g., Boykoff and Boykoff 2004). Using loaded language may invite such media coverage by giving the impression that the inclusion of an opposing view is needed for a well-balanced story.

Another problem with promotional communication by scientists is that they are seldom trained in how to appropriately or effectively communicate. Accordingly, their choices may be counterproductive to their intended outcomes. Invasive species and climate change scientists still tend to resort to fear-based language about negative impacts of nonnative species or climate disasters, even though there is evidence that such language is ineffective or even counterproductive (Nerlich et al. 2010, Larson 2011, Kahan et al. 2012). To change environmental attitudes, a focus on solutions and cultural values—as opposed to danger and loss—is often more effective. As another example, Pigliucci and Boudry (2011) argued that the use of machine-information metaphors in biology, such as was discussed above for synthetic biology, can play into the hand of proponents of intelligent design, because they “bolster design-like misconceptions about living systems” (p. 454) that are pivotal to the intelligent design argumentation of creationists.

Engage early with audiences

There is now widespread recognition among communication scholars that the limiting step for greater scientific understanding is not simply more science communication to inform the public (Bubela et al. 2009, Kahan 2010, Kahan et al. 2012), as many scientists tend to assume (Peters 2013). This view is known as the deficit model of the public understanding of science, and it has been discounted for numerous reasons, not least that knowledge is only one element of how people come to decisions. This realization does not devalue effective science communication but, instead, places it in a more appropriate context, as part of a broader suite of engagement with the public about the production and use of scientific knowledge. Evolving models of science communication incorporate citizens much earlier in the process, even insofar as designing a research program itself (Bubela et al. 2009, Kueffer et al. 2012). Interactions between scientists and stakeholders can help improve our understanding of a problem and help determine how it should be framed, formulated, and integrated into the sociocultural environment. Scientists should therefore engage earlier with diverse audiences through participatory processes to facilitate the effective use of metaphors, whether in the context of basic or applied research (see Larson 2011), and to reduce the risk of pervasive outcomes in science communication such as those mentioned above.

Many terms used in ecology and environmental studies, such as disturbance, ecosystem health, restoration, and wildlife, imply a preferred state of nature and are, therefore, implicitly normative (Carolan 2006, Larson 2011). Recently, new terms such as novel ecosystem and Anthropocene have gained prominence in biodiversity conservation to denote the dominant role of humans in nature. These new terms shift the normative stance within the field and, indeed, divide experts and stakeholders who feel that they are at opposing ends of a contested issue: Is the pervasive human presence in nature bad or good? Some conservationists fear that these terms could endorse human domination of the planet (Crist 2013) and could exacerbate the shifting cognitive baseline whereby humans tend to become accustomed to new and often degraded ecosystems and thus forget the nature of the past (Caro et al. 2012). Conversely, others propose that associating widespread anthropogenic ecosystems with positive values will be the only way to ensure that people will still care for nature and the ecosystems around them (Marris E 2011). But this is not really about choosing between two options: It is, instead, a complex and ambiguous question that touches on fundamental worldviews. It should, therefore, be seen as an issue requiring the engagement of the whole conservation community (Kueffer and Kaiser-Bunbury 2014). Such questions about how we put our scientific concerns into a cultural context cannot, therefore, be left to biologists alone but must be elaborated with the assistance of partners from the social sciences and humanities, including ethicists (Rose et al. 2012, Sörlin 2012), as well as the engaged public (Yung et al. 2013).

Toward responsible and balanced science communication

We have argued that scientists should—with the help of journalists and social scientists—find ways to communicate (often uncertain) results in an engaging way that explicitly attends to its metaphors and values. How can the situation be improved in practice? We certainly do not suggest that scientists can or should abandon metaphorical and engaging language in science communication. Instead, we propose that they adopt criteria to help distinguish problematic from fruitful ways of using such language (box 1), although this distinction will be sensitive to the scientific and societal context, must evolve over time, and will never be perfect. Therefore, we also propose procedural guidelines and training for scientists, science journalists, and the editors of scientific journals to work collaboratively to report scientific results using frames and metaphors that speak to diverse experts and stakeholders (box 2). Scientists and journalists must continuously strive to get both the facts and the story straight, because it is as unscientific to communicate with inappropriate language as it is to ­present bad data.

Box 1. Criteria for assessing the appropriateness of metaphors for science communication.

Factual correctness

Every metaphor simplifies by illustrating certain aspects of a scientific object while neglecting others. Scientific metaphors can, nonetheless, be interpreted in terms of their factual content, and, in this respect, they can be considered wrong. At the start of the genomic era, for instance, Avise (2001) proposed alternative genetic metaphors to replace prior mechanistic ones (e.g., the blueprint metaphor) that he felt misrepresented new insights about the nature of the genome (also see our discussion of synthetic biology above). Metaphors should be consistent with the state of knowledge to the degree of scientific accuracy required in a particular context (e.g., research, popular science writing, science-based decisionmaking).

Socially acceptable language

The same rules that apply to everyday life concerning socially acceptable language also apply to science. Metaphors that are racist, sexist, or in other ways offensive should be avoided. Herbers (2007), for example, condemns references to slavemaking and negro ants and reference to rape in animal behavior studies.

Neutrality

It is often difficult to assess the neutrality of a metaphor. Scientists should, nonetheless, seek in their communication to avoid language that is generally recognized to be loaded with emotion, such as apocalyptic warnings and dramatic hyperbole. This language can detract from the perceived neutrality of a scientist, who is expected to present research results that invite open and critical discussion. One rhetorical function of such metaphors is to convince when evidence is missing or ambiguous; however, this is inadvisable, insofar as it leads to scientific statements’ being supported with rhetoric instead of facts.

Transparency

When a metaphor is used, it should be introduced as such, and its connection with specific aspects of a scientific concept should be illustrated. At least in longer texts, authors should explicitly reflect on the connotations and performativity of their chosen metaphors. When metaphors are replaced by similes (i.e., using an X is like Y statement), there is a lower risk that they will be taken literally (Carolan 2006).

Box 2. Procedural guidelines and training for scientists and science journalists.

Collaboration between scientists and journalists

Close and continuous interaction between scientists and journalists is needed in the formulation of news stories, not only to get the facts right but also to ensure that the stories are well balanced, honest, and compelling to a culturally diverse audience. When new stories are written, journalists and editors are under enormous time pressure, and they are constrained by existing news frames and expectations (Smith 2005). Rhetorical stereotypes will be difficult to replace at those times. Long-term partnerships among journalists, editors, and scientists are therefore critical and may be facilitated by regular interactive workshops.

Humanities education for modern biologists

It is the responsibility of scientists to understand the historical, cultural, and normative connotations of key terms and concepts that they use (see our discussion of invasive species above). Therefore, closer collaborations between scientists and humanists are essential (Sörlin 2012, Forêt et al. 2014). To this end, graduate students in biology should take courses on the broader social and cultural context of scientific research, including science communication and the science–policy interface (Sörlin 2012, Forêt et al. 2014). Training in professional ethics should go beyond ethical practices within research (e.g., animal welfare, fraud) to address larger questions about the socioethical implications of research (Kitcher 2004).

Diversity of communicators and metaphors

In some phases of research, a focal metaphor can help direct inquiry, but, at other times, it may stymie it. In these instances, a plurality of metaphors may help inspire new ideas and thus enhance scientific inquiry and communication (e.g., Avise 2001). These metaphors can originate through interactions with people of diverse backgrounds (both scientific and nonscientific). It is laudable, for example, that scientists increasingly engage with artists about their research and how to communicate it (e.g., Scott 2006).

Peer review of rhetoric and metaphors

Our recommendations apply not just to public communication but also to original scientific articles. We welcome the more creative and engaging style of many contemporary scientific articles, but scientists should be aware that these rhetorical flourishes can be problematic and misleading (Nelkin 1994, Larson 2011). During peer review, referees should check not only the content of articles but also the language and metaphors used to communicate and illustrate concepts and arguments. This requires that journal editors extend their review guidelines accordingly to improve referees’ awareness of these issues.

Democratic deliberation and audience studies

Metaphors influence the type of inquiry that is pursued in both basic and applied research, so their selection will often have sociopolitical implications. Participatory processes can help increase democratic deliberation about metaphors in research in order to legitimize their use and to ameliorate their outcomes for science and society. For instance, focused communication campaigns rely on audience studies that are intended to clarify the implications of particular value-laden metaphors for communication, such as those that the National Academy of Sciences has used for communication about evolution (Nisbet 2009).

We thank Brigitte Nerlich, Erle Ellis, Franziska Humair, Juanita Schlaepfer-Miller, and several anonymous reviewers for comments and suggestions on earlier versions of this article.

References cited

Avise
JC.
Evolving genomic metaphors: A new look at the language of DNA
Science
2001
294
86
87

Beardsley
TM.
Framing biology
BioScience
2006
56
555

Boudry
M
Pigliucci
M
The mismeasure of machine: Synthetic biology and the trouble with engineering metaphors
Studies in History and Philosophy of Science C
2013
44
660
668

Boykoff
MT
Boykoff
JM
Balance as bias: Global warming and the US prestige press
Global Environmental Change
2004
14
125
136

Brown
TL.
Making Truth: Metaphor in Science
2003
University of Illinois Press

Bubela
T
et al. 
Science communication reconsidered
Nature Biotechnology
2009
27
514
518

Caro
T
Darwin
J
Forrester
T
Ledoux-Bloom
C
Wells
C
Conservation in the Anthropocene
Conservation Biology
2012
26
185
188

Carolan
MS.
The values and vulnerabilities of metaphors within the environmental sciences
Society and Natural Resources
2006
19
921
930

Courchamp
F.
Alien species: Monster fern makes the IUCN invader list
Nature
2013
498
37
doi:10.1038/498037a

Crist
E.
On the poverty of our nomenclature
Environmental Humanities
2013
3
129
147

Cserer
A
Seiringer
A
Pictures of synthetic biology
Systems and Synthetic Biology
2009
3
27
35

Darwin
C.
On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or The Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life
1869
5th ed
Murray

Deplazes-Zemp
A.
The conception of life in synthetic biology
Science and Engineering Ethics
2012
18
757
774

Fischer
J
Ritchie
EG
Hanspach
J
Academia's obsession with quantity
Trends in Ecology and Evolution
2012
27
473
474

Forêt
P
Hall
M
Kueffer
C
Developing the environmental humanities: A Swiss perspective
Gaia
2014
23
67
69

Groffman
PM
Stylinski
C
Nisbet
MC
Duarte
CM
Jordan
R
Burgin
A
Previtali
MA
Coloso
J
Restarting the conversation: Challenges at the interface between ecology and society
Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment
2010
8
284
291

Hamblin
JD.
Arming Mother Nature: The Birth of Catastrophic Environmentalism
2013
Oxford University Press

Hamilton
G.
Welcome weeds: How alien invasion could save the Earth
New Scientist
2011
2795
34
37

Hellsten
I
Nerlich
B
Synthetic biology: Building the language for a new science brick by metaphorical brick
New Genetics and Society
2011
30
375
397

Herbers
JM.
Watch your language! Racially loaded metaphors in scientific research
BioScience
2007
57
104
105

Horeis
H.
Once upon a time, there was a dying forest
Technology in Society
2009
31
111
116

Humair
F
Edwards
PJ
Siegrist
M
Kueffer
C
Understanding misunderstandings in invasion science: Why experts don't agree on common concepts and risk assessments
Neobiota
2014
20
1
30

Jacob
F.
Evolution and tinkering
Science
1977
196
1161
1166

Kahan
D[M]
Fixing the communications failure
Nature
2010
463
296
297

Kahan
DM
Peters
E
Wittlin
M
Slovic
P
Ouellette
LL
Braman
D
Mandel
G
The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks
Nature Climate Change
2012
2
732
735

Keller
EF.
Making Sense of Life: Explaining Biological Development with Models, Metaphors, and Machines
2002
Harvard University Press

Keller
EF.
What does synthetic biology have to do with biology?
BioSocieties
2009
4
291
302

Kitcher
P.
Responsible biology
BioScience
2004
54
331
336

Kuchner
MJ.
Marketing for Scientists: How to Shine in Tough Times
2012
Island Press

Kueffer
C
Kaiser-Bunbury
CN
Reconciling conflicting perspectives for biodiversity conservation in the Anthropocene
Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment
2014
12
131
137

Kueffer
C
et al. 
Enabling effective problem-oriented research for sustainable development
Ecology and Society
2012
17
(art. 8)

Larson
BMH.
The war of the roses: demilitarizing invasion biology
Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment
2005
3
495
500

Larson
BMH.
Optimizing friction between alternative genomic metaphors: How much plurality is enough?
Genomics, Society and Policy
2009
5
20
28

Larson
BMH.
Metaphors for Environmental Sustainability: Redefining our Relationship with Nature
2011
Yale University Press

Larson
BMH
Kueffer
C
ZiF Working Group on Ecological Novelty
Managing invasive species amidst high uncertainty and novelty
Trends in Ecology and Evolution
2013
28
255
256

Marris
C
Rose
N
Let's get real on synthetic biology
New Scientist
2012
2868
28
29

Marris
E.
Rambunctious Garden: Saving Nature in a Post-Wild World
Bloomsbury
2011

Nelder
C.
Communication: Positive energy
Nature
2013
498
293
295

Nelkin
D.
Promotional metaphors and their popular appeal
Public Understanding of Science
1994
3
25
31

Nerlich
B
Elliot
R
Larson
B[MH]
Communicating Biological Sciences: Ethical and Metaphorical Dimensions
2009
Ashgate

Nerlich
B
Koteyko
N
Brown
B
Theory and language of climate change communication
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change
2010
1
97
110

Nisbet
MC.
Nerlich
B
Elliott
R
Larson
B[MH]
The ethics of framing science
Communicating Biological Sciences: Ethical and Metaphorical Dimensions
2009
Ashgate
51
74

Peters
HP.
Gap between science and media revisited: Scientists as public communicators
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
2013
110
suppl. 3
14102
14109

Pielke
RA
Jr
The Honest Broker: Making Sense of Science in Policy and Politics
2007
Cambridge University Press

Pigliucci
M
Boudry
M
Why machine-information metaphors are bad for science and science education
Science and Education
2011
20
453
471

Proctor
JD
Larson
BMH
Ecology, complexity, and metaphor
BioScience
2005
55
1065
1068

Raymond
CM
Singh
GG
Benessaiah
K
Bernhardt
JR
Levine
J
Nelson
H
Turner
NJ
Norton
B
Tam
J
Chan
KMA
Ecosystem services and beyond: Using multiple metaphors to understand human-environment relationships
BioScience
2013
63
536
546

Rose
DB
van Dooren
T
Chrulew
M
Cooke
S
Kearnes
M
O’Gormand
E
Thinking through the environment, unsettling the humanities
Environmental Humanities
2012
1
1
5

Sarewitz
D.
How science makes environmental controversies worse
Environmental Science and Policy
2004
7
385
403

Schlaepfer
MA
Sax
DF
Olden
JD
The potential conservation value of non-native species
Conservation Biology
2011
25
428
437

Scott
J
Artists-in-Labs: Processes of Inquiry
2006
Springer

Slaney
KL
Maraun
MD
Analogy and metaphor running amok: An examination of the use of explanatory devices in neuroscience
Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
2005
25
153
172

Smith
J.
Dangerous news: Media decision making about climate change risk
Risk Analysis
2005
25
1471
1482

Sontag
S.
AIDS and its Metaphors
1989
Farrar, Straus, and Giroux

Sörlin
S.
Environmental humanities: Why should biologists interested in the environment take the humanities seriously?
BioScience
2012
62
788
789

Vince
G.
Embracing invasives
Science
2011
331
1383
1384

Weber
JR
Word
CS
The communication process as evaluative context: What do nonscientists hear when scientists speak?
BioScience
2001
51
487
495

Young
AM
Larson
BMH
Clarifying debates in invasion biology: A survey of invasion biologists
Environmental Research
2011
111
893
898

Yung
L
Schwarze
S
Carr
W
Chapin
FS
III
Marris
E
Hobbs
RJ
Higgs
ES
Hall
CM
Engaging the public in novel ecosystems
Novel Ecosystems: Intervening in the New Ecological World Order
2013
Wiley-Blackwell
247
256